14 April 2014

Climate change communication: Key psychological research findings (and why you haven't heard about them yet) (2)

Research has identified a number of psychological barriers that can prevent people from believing in or acting on messages about climate change. Luckily, it has also suggested strategies for overcoming these barriers. Second of a two-part report by Paul Connor.

Second of 2 parts | Part 1

5. Some messages can get through to conservatives! Sort of…

One of the most common analyses one hears about the social psychology of climate change is that the issue has become increasingly politicised over the last decade. More and more, it is said, people are making up their minds on the issue according to their political allegiance, and not by an objective assessment of the facts. And certainly, there has been an observable trend for opinions on the issue to increasingly diverge across political and ideological lines.
 But even with this increasing polarisation, the party line split in climate change attitudes is not absolute. In both Australia and the US, research shows that around one in four conservative voters still accepts the basic tenets of climate change science that climate change is happening and that it is due to human activities. And there is some pretty good research out there that suggests tailoring your climate change messages in the right way can increase their appeal to conservative audiences.

In their 2010 paper entitled ‘System Justification, the Denial of Global Warming, and the Possibility of ''System-Sanctioned Change''’, Irina Feygina of New York University and colleagues explored the connection between attitudes towards environmental issues and a constellation of beliefs known as ‘system justification’. System justification is the tendency in some individuals to defend society’s status quo and see the way things are as right and just. It is strongly related to conservative political attitudes, and is thought to stem from a psychological need for order, structure, and security.

Feygina and colleagues began by using large samples of undergrad students (340 in the first study and 563 in the second) to compile correlational data regarding system justification and environmental attitudes. Oddly, given their paper’s title, they did not look specifically at climate change denial, but focused more broadly on denial of environmental issues in general, which they measured by asking participants how much they agreed with statements such as “The so-called “ecological crisis” facing humankind has been greatly exaggerated”.

In both experiments, they found system justification and environmental denial to be strongly related. In fact, in the second study, they found that the extent to which people reported holding system justifying beliefs largely (but not totally) explained the connection between people’s political orientation and their denial of environmental problems.

Feygina and colleagues’ third study was experimental. First, they presented 41 undergrads with a generic message about research into the environment. E.g., “Today, researchers are especially interested in the relationship between people and the environment”. They then presented some of the participants with what they called a ‘system-sanctioned’ message designed to make pro-environmental behaviour more appealing to system justifiers. It read: “Being pro-environmental allows us to protect and preserve the American way of life. It is patriotic to conserve the country’s natural resources”. The participants’ then answered questions regarding their intentions to act pro-environmentally, and also about their system-justifying beliefs. Finally, the participants were given the option of signing a number of pro-environmental petitions.

The results showed that overall, there was no significant difference between the ‘system sanctioned’ message and the normal message in promoting pro-environmental intentions and behaviours.  However, the results showed that among people high in system justification, the system sanctioned message was significantly more effective. The authors concluded that “…reframing environmentalism as supporting (rather than undermining) the American way or life eliminates the negative effect of system justification on pro-environmental behavior”.

The effect of the normal and ‘system-sanctioned’ (or system-preservation’) environmental messages over the pro-environmental intentions of low and high system justifiers in Feygina and colleagues’ results. While the system justifying message (the bold black line) was more effective for high system justifiers, the normal message (the dotted line) was more effective among low system-justifiers.
 Another study that explored ways of reframing environmental messages for a conservative audience was the 2012 study by Paul Bain from the University of Queensland and colleagues entitled “Promoting pro-environment action in climate change deniers”. Bain and colleagues were interested in whether climate change sceptics could perhaps be sold on climate change action by its possible co-benefits in addition to mitigating climate change.

To start, they recruited 155 people “from the general public” who either did not believe climate change was happening or believed it was not human caused. These people were then asked to imagine a future society where substantial action had been taken to combat climate change. After this, they were asked to rate how different that future society would be from today in terms of a number of different characteristics. These included the interpersonal warmth of the society, its societal competence, and its societal development (which involves, for example, scientific or technological advancement). They were then asked about their intentions towards performing ‘environmental citizenship’ actions. These included actions like donating to environmental groups, signing pro-environmental petitions, and voting for political candidates because of their environmental credentials. Results showed that overall, the sceptics were more likely to report higher environmental citizenship intentions when they thought that that acting on climate change would create a more interpersonally warm society, and also when they thought that it would enhance societal development.

Following on from this finding, Bain and colleagues tested whether messages aimed at promoting such co-benefits of climate change action might prove effective among climate skeptics. To do so, they recruited another 347 people from the public, including 128 skeptics, who read one of three ‘personal testimonials’ relating different reasons for supporting action on climate change. One of the testimonials suggested climate change action would create a more interpersonally warm society (e.g. “I think it’d make us more considerate in other ways – like looking out for each other, and caring for people in the community”), one suggested that climate change action would promote societal development (e.g. “Taking action to reduce energy pollution would lead to new scientific breakthroughs and new industries”), and one suggested that climate change action would prevent environmental destruction (e.g. ““We'd be less affected by food and water shortages”). As expected, the results showed that the testimonials promoting the co-benefits of climate change action produced higher levels of environmental citizenship among the sceptics than the testimonial that focused on environmental destruction. What’s more, these co-benefits testimonials were also more effective in producing environmental citizenship intentions among the climate change believers (though this effect did not reach a statistically significant level).

The effectiveness of different climate change ‘personal testimonials’ in Bain and colleagues’ study. Among climate sceptics (on the left) the message focused on societal warmth and societal development were significantly more effective than the message focused on environmental damage. Among believers (o the right) the same pattern of results was observed, but did not reach significance.
One slight caveat to these results should, however, be mentioned. In Bain and colleagues’ study, the co-benefits testimonials included an introductory statement that indicating an agnostic position towards climate science (“People like Al Gore say that reducing carbon emissions will stop the planet heating up, and most scientists seem to think that. But then there’s other scientists who think that’s not true”). Yet in the environmental destruction testimonial, the introductory statement expressed a more dogmatic position regarding climate science (“People like Al Gore say that reducing carbon emissions will stop the planet heating up, and most scientists seem to think that. I think that because there is general consensus among scientists we have to accept that climate change is real”). This, then, created another confounding variable. It may be that it was not the discussions of the co-benefits that caused their testimonials to be more effective among sceptics. Instead, this may have related to their agnosticism regarding climate science being perceived as less alienating among the sceptics.

Despite this quibble, however, both these studies indicate that climate activists need not necessarily give up on conservative-minded individuals. Instead, they indicate that there is room for tailored messaging towards conservative audiences, which keeps in mind the arguments that will appeal to more conservative mindsets, and remembers that even climate skeptics can sometimes see non-environmental benefits in climate change action.

5. Emphasise the scientific consensus!

Another message that seems to be important for winning support for climate change policies is that of the consensus among climate scienists. 97% of climate scientists agree with the consensus position that the planet is warming and we are causing it, yet studies have shown that people generally misunderstand this to a surprising degree.

A 2011 study by Ding Ding of the University of California San Diego and colleagues entitled ‘Support for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific agreement’ examined this phenomenon. The study asked 751 US participants to report their perceptions of the scientific consensus regarding climate change, their own beliefs regarding climate change, and their support for climate change policies.

The results showed that there was both a huge misunderstanding of the scientific consensus, with 66% of the respondents classified as ‘consensus not understood’. These were people who agreed with one of the following statements: ‘there is a lot of disagreement among scientists about whether or not global warming is happening’ (45%), ‘most scientists think that global warming is not happening’ (5%) or they ‘do not know enough to say’ (16%). The results also showed that there was a strong correlation between people’s perceptions of the scientific consensus around climate change and their support for climate change policies. In fact, people’s perceptions of the scientific consensus were shown to explain nearly 60% of the variance in their support for climate policies (explaining variance basically means how well one thing can be predicted from another, in this case how well we can predict people’s support or climate policies if we just know about their perception of the scientific consensus).

The authors also performed a mediation analysis. This basically involves checking if your data matches a particular theory about one variable having an effect over another variable via a third mediating variable (For example, the correlation between owning drums and being disliked by neighbours is very likely mediated by a third variable – the overall amount of drums played). They proposed that people’s perceptions of the consensus were affecting the degree to which they held certain key beliefs about climate change (including whether climate change is happening and whether it is human caused), which was in turn affecting their support for climate change policies. Their data supported this theory (though keep in mind this data is correlational!).

Extending upon Ding and colleague’s findings, Stephan Lewandowsky of the University of Bristol and colleagues sought to experimentally investigate the effects of scientific consensus information in their 2012 paper entitled ‘The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science’. Surveying people recruited from the street (in Perth I believe, though I’m not sure), they first asked people how many out of 100 climate scientists they believed endorsed the belief that climate change is happening and caused by humans. They then provided information about the 97% scientific consensus to half of these people via a short text and graphs (the other half received a ‘neutral’ message about climate change – I do not know what that was though!), and following this asked people about a number of key beliefs about climate change. Finally, they asked participants about how much they endorsed free market capitalism, a view previously shown to be related to lower belief in climate change.

An example of ‘consensus information’ being used in climate communication research, Please note that this was provided by another researcher, not Stephen Lewandowsky, but that it has been used and has achieved equivalent effects to the results discussed from Lewandowsky and colleagues.
   Like Ding and colleagues’ study, results showed that people vastly underestimated the consensus among climate scientists, with the average guess having 67% of climate scientists accepting the consensus position. Yet providing people with consensus information had a large effect. The group that received the consensus information showed significantly higher belief in climate change than the group not provided the consensus information, and the difference between the two groups was of a size rarely seen in research of this kind. In addition, among the group who did not receive the consensus information, endorsement of free market capitalism was related with lower belief in climate change. Yet crucially, among the people who received the consensus information, there was no significant relationship between endorsement of free market capitalism and belief in climate change. The authors concluded that “the role of ideology was drastically attenuated when participants were provided with information about the scientific consensus”.


Results from Lewandowsky and colleagues’ 2012 study. While in the control condition higher free market beliefs were associated with lower belief in climate change (AGW), when consensus information was provided this association was absent.
While Lewandowsky only examined the effects of consensus information on climate beliefs and not on behavioural intentions or policy support, he and others (including John Cook of the University of Queensland and hopefully myself!) are continuing to perform research on the effects of consensus information. And given the previously established links between climate beliefs, behaviour and policy support, it is likely that this work will again show strong effects of consensus information. So although it is early days, these results certainly seem important. My advice, then, is to provide consensus information whenever possible, and  watch this space! 
7. Make people feel good about themselves.

The last study I want to discuss is the 2010 paper by Paul Sparks and colleagues from the University of Sussex entitled ‘Pro-environmental actions, climate change,
and defensiveness: Do self-affirmations make a difference to people's motives and beliefs about making a difference?’. It’s a nice paper. Not only is it solid scientifically, but I also think it says something pretty poignant about individuals need to be approached by change makers.

Sparks and colleagues were interested in investigating the effect of ‘self-affirmation’ on climate change denial and motivations towards pro-environmental behaviours. The theory behind self-affirmation is basically about how we protect out concept of our self. It suggests that when we feel more sure of ourselves and confident of our own self-image, we are generally more open to receiving new information, and react less defensively to information that is threatening or upsetting in some way.

In Sparks and colleagues’ first experiment, 125 undergrad students were asked to complete either a self-affirmation exercise or a non self-affirming questionnaire about themselves. The self-affirming exercise, designed to strengthen the participants’ perceptions of themselves as being kind, asked the participants to list instances when they had behaved kindly or compassionately towards others. The non self-affirming questionnaire simply asked neutral questions about the participants, such their favourite flavour of ice cream. Following this, all participants were presented with a number of real life climate change communications chosen by the researchers for their ‘threatening’ nature. One was taken from the Guardian newspaper, one from a book by George Monbiot, and one was this gloomy missive from James Lovelock: “Why are we so slow . . . to see the great peril that faces us and civilization? What stops us from realizing that the fever of global heating is real and deadly and might already have moved outside our and the Earth's control?”. After reading these messages, the participants were asked about their beliefs regarding climate change.

Results showed that the participants who had completed the self-affirmation exercise reported lower levels of denial regarding climate change than non-affirmed participants. Interestingly, this ‘denial’ was mainly due to what the researchers called ‘denial of self involvement’, measured by participants’ agreement with statements such as “My own personal impact upon climate change is insignificant”. While the affirmed participants also reported lower levels of ‘denial of outcome severity’, which was measured by their agreement with statements such as “The threat of climate change has been exaggerated”, this difference was not quite statistically significant.

Sparks and colleagues then used another 90 undergrads to investigat whether self-affirmation would also affect people’s reactions to messages encouraging pro-environmental behaviours such as recycling. This time, their self-affirmed group simply chose a particular value from a list (e.g. kindness, forgiveness, altruism) that they felt was most important to them, while the non-affirmed group did nothing. Following this, information was presented to all the participants about the risks of not recycling, and attitudes and intentions towards recycling were measured.

The results again showed that the affirmation exercise made a difference. Among people who had previously been relatively low in recycling behaviours, the self-affirmation produced significantly higher intentions to recycle in the future. The researchers concluded that “…both studies point to the success of manipulations of self-affirmation and point to further avenues where self-affirmation theory may be potentially applied to the psychology of environmental issues”.

It appears, then, that when people feel more secure and confident about who they are and the values they hold, they are more open to potentially threatening climate change and environmental messages, and also potentially more open to changing their behaviour. Again, when I read this finding I was reminded of an activist I had once seen intuitively applying it in her work. Speaking on television about the need to move away from fossil fuels such as coal, she prefaced everything she said by praising the hard working and self-sacrificing coal communities that had so reliably toiled to provide energy for our society for the past century. Given the threatening nature of her message for those communities, such an affirmation of their worth and moral integrity was no doubt a wise strategy.

I have to say, I do find it kind of amazing that asking someone to list a few occasions they have been kind or choose a value that is important to them could have an effect over their reactions to climate change messages or their intentions to recycle. Yet it did. And this, I suppose, is yet another reminder of a common theme that runs through much of the research discussed here – climate change can simply be confronting to a lot of different people in a lot of different ways. Whether through threatening just world beliefs, through threatening system justification beliefs, through making people feel disempowered, or through threatening people’s sense of self, there are just a lot of ways that people can and will find messages about climate change psychologically aversive. And this is obviously a major challenge for both the activists and social psychologists working in the area. But as I hope I have shown, there are some ways of using evidence-based knowledge regarding these challenges in order to more ably navigate them.

References

Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., Bongiorno, R., & Jeffries, C. (2012a). Promoting pro-environmental action in climate change deniers. Nature Climate Change, 2, 600-603.
Ding, D., Maibach, E. W., Zhao, X., Roser-Renouf, R., & Leiserowitx, A. (2011). Support for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific agreement. Nature Climate Change, 1, 462-466.
Feygina, I., Jost, J. T., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2010). System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of “system- sanctioned change.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(3), 326–338.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3, 399=404.
Sparks, P., Jessop, D. C., Chapman, J., & Holmes, K. (2010). Pro-environmental actions, climate change, and defensiveness: Do self-affirmations make a difference to people’s motives and beliefs about making a difference? British     Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 553-568.


Paul Connor is a climate change campaigner and social psychological researcher at the University of Melbourne. He blogs at www.paulconnor.org.